"PERCEIVED LOVE AND ACCEPTANCE" AS AN ELEMENT OF SELF-ESTEEM: AN EXPLORATORY STUDY A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of the Department of Psychology San Jose State University In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts by Mark Brautigam August, 1984
APPROVED FOR THE DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY __________________________________________ Dr. Milton Andersen, Committee Chairperson __________________________________________ Dr. Frank Payne __________________________________________ Dr. Gene Medinnus __________________________________________ Dr. W. T. Plant, Department Chairperson APPROVED FOR THE UNIVERSITY __________________________________________
TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 METHOD . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 Subjects . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11 RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 Acceptance Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 Virtue Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17 Shyness Scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18 Differences based on sex, class level, age . . . . 19 Self-esteem scale interrelationships . . . . . . . 19 Self-esteem and related variables . . . . . . . . 24 Interview data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34 Dimensions of Self-esteem . . . . . . . . . . . . 34 Development of Self-esteem . . . . . . . . . . . . 36 Directions for future research . . . . . . . . . . 39 Final note . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 SUMMARY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43
REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44 APPENDICES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 Appendix A: Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale . . . . . 47 Appendix B: Self-Efficacy Scale . . . . . . . . . 48 Appendix C: Texas Social Behavior Inventory . . . 50 Appendix D: Items for Love and Acceptance Scale . 52 Appendix E: Items for Virtue Scale . . . . . . . 54 Appendix F: Stanford Shyness Survey . . . . . . . 55 Appendix G: Parental Interest Index . . . . . . . 57 Appendix H: UCLA Loneliness Scale . . . . . . . . 58 Appendix J: Cover Sheet . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 Appendix K: Informed Consent for Interview . . . 61
LIST OF TABLES TABLE PAGE 1 Item-total correlations and alpha coefficients for 10-item acceptance scale . . . . . . . . . . . 15 2 Factor pattern of the 10-item scale rotated to an oblique solution . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16 3 Correlation of global self-esteem scale with self-esteem components . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20 4 Correlations between self-esteem components . . . 20 5 Correlation of Self-esteem measures with virtue pool items . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21 6 Factor analysis of four self-esteem measures rotated to Kaiser's Varimax criterion . . . . . . 23 7 Correlation of Self-esteem measures with Parental Interest Index items . . . . . . . . . . 24 8 Correlation between loneliness and various measures of self-esteem . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 9 Factor pattern of Acceptance and Loneliness items rotated to an oblique solution . . . . . . . 26 10 Correlation between shyness and various measures of self-esteem . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27 11 Correlations between scale scores and oral self-ratings . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 12 Items contributing to good self-esteem as discussed in personal interviews with university students . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30 13 Items contributing to poor self-esteem as discussed in personal interviews with university students . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31 14 Number of items contributing to good self-esteem and poor self-esteem categorized by area . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32
15 How 23 university students ranked four areas as contributing to their good self-esteem or poor self-esteem . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 16 Number of students who selected various virtue pool items as being relevant to their self-esteem . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36
1 INTRODUCTION The study of self-esteem is justified by the fact that poor self-esteem has been shown to be predictive of (and perhaps antecedent to) neurosis (Rosenberg, 1979), physical illness (Thomas, 1982), and social maladjustment (Jacobs, Bersheid, & Walster, 1971); good self-esteem has been shown to insulate against the harmful effects of life stressors on physical health (Petrie & Rotheram, 1982). Also, and perhaps more importantly, poor self-esteem can be the source of considerable emotional pain; this is demonstrated by the correlation between poor self-esteem and depression (Rosenberg, 1965, 1979; Wilson & Krane, 1980; Battle, 1980). To help people overcome the burden of poor self-esteem is a necessary task; but in order to do so, it is essential that the nature of poor self-esteem be clearly understood. Self- esteem has been defined as self-regard or self-worth (Rosenberg, 1979). However, most measures of self-esteem seem to measure "general" self-esteem, consisting of several factors which are not distinguished; others measure only the self-perception of task competence or social competence. It is the purpose of this exploratory study to show that the individual's feeling of acceptance or rejection is an important element of self-esteem which has been heretofore neglected, and to introduce a scale for the measurement of
2 this construct. Considering self-esteem as a global construct consisting of several component factors is not a new idea. Gecas (1971) was the first to suggest differentiating between different types of self-esteem. His study of the effects of parental support and control on the self-esteem of adolescents identified two distinct factors which together constituted global self-esteem: a "power" factor and a "worth" factor. "Power" refers to the individual's feelings of competence, effectiveness, and personal influence; "worth," on the other hand, refers to feelings of personal virtue and moral worth. In a more recent study, DeGregorio and Carver (1980) found it expedient to distinguish between "instrumental" self-esteem (evaluation of the self as a goal achiever) and "social" self-esteem. Coopersmith (1967) saw several different areas from which individuals might potentially derive self-esteem. They are four criteria people commonly use to define their success: (1) Power, the ability to influence and control others; (2) Significance, the acceptance, attention, and affection of others; (3) Virtue, adherence to ethical and moral standards; and (4) Competence, successful achievement and performance. Lasky (1979) rated her subjects subjectively in these four areas (on the basis of private interviews). She asserted that those with the highest self-
3 esteem felt both significant (loved and accepted) and competent (successful in their careers); those with low self-esteem were lacking in one or both of these areas. In other words, for some, poor self-esteem consisted of feeling unloved rather than feeling incompetent. This shows that the relationship we will find between self-esteem and other variables depends a great deal on how we define self-esteem. The problem is that apart from Lasky's (1979) study, in which the ratings were made subjectively and post-hoc, no study has seriously addressed the "significance" dimension of self-esteem (which, for the sake of clarity, will hereafter be referred to as "perceived love and acceptance"). If, as Rosenberg (1965) suggests, self-esteem is indeed a product of the interest shown in a child by his or her parents, rather than a product of experiences of success, then it seems logical to consider "perceived love and acceptance," rather than "competence," to be the most important element of self- esteem; and to consider the affect associated with poor self- esteem to be the pain of rejection, rather than the shame of incompetence. Other studies indicate that "perceived love and acceptance" is an important element of self-esteem as predictive of social behavior. Jacobs, Berscheid, and Walster (1971) showed that people with poor self-esteem are unusually receptive to affection but have a hard time
4 recognizing when it is being offered. Thus it might be said that people with poor self-esteem are hungry for affection, but are accustomed only to being rejected. Concerning self- esteem and shyness, Pilkonis and Zimbardo (1979) constructed a scenario in which (1) conditioned anxiety to social stimuli motivates (2) avoidance behaviors which (3) prevent the learning of appropriate social skills and (4) produce expectations of failure in social situations. These expectations of failure "may elevate the original anxiety and encourage avoidance in an escalating negative-loop cycle" (Pilkonis & Zimbardo, 1979, p. 136). But what conditions the original anxiety? "Aversive social interactions" -- e.g., experiences of rejection. It is apparent that in studying both the antecedents and the social consequences of self- esteem, "perceived love and acceptance" is the real issue. This study investigated the relationships between self- esteem and its primary antecedent, parental interest (Rosenberg, 1965), and between self-esteem and one of its social consequences, shyness (Pilkonis & Zimbardo, 1979). However, rather than using one measure of self-esteem, five measures were used. One was a measure of "general" self- esteem. The other four measures corresponded to Coopersmith's (1967) four criteria for defining success, which have been renamed (for the sake of clarity) as follows: (1) Social Confidence, (2) Perceived Love and Acceptance, (3)
5 Virtue, and (4) Competence. In each case, self-report measures were used, because it was assumed that no one is more acquainted with the subject's self-perceptions than the subject himself (or herself). The area of acceptance and rejection was expected to correlated with loneliness. Perlman and Peplau (1981) have shown that lonely adolescents reported a higher incidence of being rejected by their parents. Russell, Peplau, and Cutrona (1980) showed that scores on the Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale correlated significantly with "feeling abandoned." Therefore, a scale measuring loneliness was included in the study. It was hoped that acceptance- rejection would correlate moderately with loneliness, but low enough to confirm the discriminant validity of the perceived love and acceptance scale. In addition to written self-report measures, there was also opportunity for oral self-reports; i.e., some of the students were interviewed. The interviews were important for three reasons. First, the investigator has adopted an ethogenic approach to social psychological research (Harre, 1977). This approach requires that subjects must not simply act, but must account for their actions. In this study, the "act" consists of selecting a particular response on a written self-report inventory, while the "account" is the subject's explanation of his or her reasons for selecting
6 that particular response. Different subjects may have different reasons for selecting the same response; and only by requiring them to account for their actions do we really begin to understand their behavior. The second reason for the interviews is that in a study such as this, where it is sought to validate a new construct, the subjects must be given an opportunity to make a wider range of responses than allowed by a forced-choice measure. Poor self-esteem will be truly understood only as people are allowed to explain it in their own phenomenological terms. The third reason for the interviews is to serve as a validity check on the written self-report measures.
7 METHOD Subjects Subjects were 146 students in six undergraduate psychology classes at San Jose State University. Fifty identified themselves as males, 89 as females; seven failed to indicate their gender. They represented many major fields of study, including psychology, engineering, business, the sciences, the arts, and education. These students ranged in age from 18 to 43 years old. Thirty-four of these students volunteered to be interviewed by the investigator. Eleven of these either changed their minds, failed to arrive at their scheduled time, or could not be scheduled. The remaining 23 students, six males and 17 females, ages 18 to 43 years, were interviewed by the investigator over a period of one week, on the university campus, at various times between their classes. Measures (1) General self-esteem was measured by the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965), a Guttman scale consisting of ten likert-type items. Test-retest reliability for this instrument is high (r = .85) (Silber & Tippett, 1965). Construct validity is substantiated by significant negative correlations with depressive affect and anxiety, and
8 positive correlations with peer-group reputation (Rosenberg, 1979). Factor analysis reveals only two factors in the scale: "positive self-esteem" and "negative self-esteem" (Carmines & Zeller, 1974). Although the instrument was constructed as a Guttman scale, Robinson and Shaver (1973) have shown that it can more easily and with equal confidence be scored as a ten-item likert scale. When scored as Rosenberg (1965) suggests, the scale correlates .59 with Coopersmith's (1967) Self-Esteem Inventory; when scored as a likert scale, the correlation is .60 (Robinson & Shaver, 1973). The Self-Esteem Scale appears in Appendix A. (2) The first component of self-esteem, "competence," was measured by the Self-Efficacy Scale (Sherer, Maddux, Mercandante, Prentice-Dunn, Jacobs, & Rogers, 1982), a measure of personal mastery expectations. Its 23 items include two subscales, of which only one, "general self- efficacy", consisting of 16 items, was used. Cronbach alpha reliability of this subscale is high (r = .86). Criterion validity is attested by the strong relationship of scale scores to history of vocational, educational, and military success (Sherer et al., 1982). The Self-Efficacy Scale appears in Appendix B. (3) The second component of self-esteem, "social confidence," was measured by a short form of the Texas Social Behavior Inventory (Helmreich & Stapp, 1974). Reportedly
9 measuring social self-esteem or social competence, it is a 16-item likert-type scale. Parallel-forms reliability of the scale is .89; scores on the short form correlate .97 with the 32-item version of the scale. Russell, Peplau, and Cutrona (1980) reported obtaining a coefficient alpha of .85 for this scale. Scores have been positively correlated with instrumentality, but not with intelligence. There is a strong relationship between scale scores and achievement of academic and other honors. This scale appears in Appendix C. (4) Since measures of "perceived love and acceptance" and "virtue" (adherence to moral and ethical standards) do not appear in the literature, likert-type scales were constructed especially for use in this study. The pool of items for the "Perceived Love and Acceptance" scale contained 21 items; the pool for the "Virtue" scale contained 10. (See Appendices D and E.) Most of the items are original. Item 18 of the Perceived Love and Acceptance Scale is from an Alienation Scale (Dean, 1961). Items 19, 20, and 21 are from an Acceptability to Others Scale (Fey, 1955). Scale construction procedures are discussed below. (5) The Stanford Shyness Survey (Zimbardo, 1977) asks the individual to state if he or she is shy or not shy; then, if shyness is the case, to report the degree of shyness, the stability of shyness over time, the extent to which shyness is negative or debilitating, and the kinds and number of
10 situations in which shyness is experienced. Zimbardo gave no instructions for scoring the survey as a scale; a method of doing so is described below. (6) "Parental Interest" will be assessed by the Parental Interest Index (Rosenberg, 1965), which consists of seven multiple-choice questions asking the subject to recall being 10-11 years old. Answers reveal the extent to which the subject remembers and perceives the parents to have shown an interest in knowing about the subject's friends and grades in school, and in listening to the subject's conversation. The Parental Interest Index does not yield a single score; therefore, responses to the items were examined individually. The Parental Interest Index appears in Appendix G. (7) Loneliness was measured using the Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (Russell, Peplau, & Cutrona, 1980). Among university students, this 20-item scale has been shown to correlate significantly with feeling "abandoned, depressed, empty, hopeless, isolated, and self-enclosed." It correlated highly with the Beck Depression Inventory, and with the Costello-Comrey Anxiety and Depression Scales. Lonely students reported having fewer friends, doing fewer social activities, and spending more time alone each day. Non- dating students were significantly lonelier than were dating or married students. Coefficient alpha for this scale was .94.
11 Procedure First questionnaire administration. 148 students completed the various scales in class as groups. (The questionnaires of two students were not included in the analysis; one was a graduate student in psychology, the other had completed the form incorrectly.) Instructions appeared on the cover sheet (Appendix J) and were also recited orally by the investigator. Two forms of the questionnaire package were used, one having the order of the scales within the package reversed, to counterbalance for order effects. Private interviews. After the test administration, the participants were made aware of the purpose of the study, although not of its expected outcome. They were then given the opportunity to volunteer to be interviewed by the investigator. It is vital to the ethogenic approach to social psychological reasearch (Harre, 1977) that the subejcts not simply be blindly exposed to various conditions, but also be allowed to freely and intelligently explain their actions in the experimental situations. The "reasons" given by subjects to explain their actions are at least as important as any inferences the investigator may make about the phenomenological "causes" of their behavior. Of 34 students who volunteered, 23 were eventually interviewed privately by the investigator. Before being
12 interviewed, each of the students was ased to sign a consent form which described the potential risks of participating in a psychological research study. None of the students declined to sign the consent form; and although each was given the option of backing out of the interview if he or she found it uncomfortable, none did so. The consent form is reproduced in Appendix K. The investigator was unaware of the test scores of the students being interviewed. The students were first asked to describe the things that made them "feel good about" themselves and the things that made them "feel not so good about" themselves; then they were asked to rank these items in terms of which made them feel "the best about" themselves and which made them feel "the worst about" themselves. Second, the investigator explained the four "component" areas of self-esteem to each of the students. Performance was described as "how well you do in school, how well you do on the job, how intelligent you consider yourself to be, how talented you are." Acceptance was described as "how much other people like you and accept you as you are." Social confidence was described as "how you get along in social situations." Ethics was described as "your personal moral standards and how you live up to them." The students were asked to rate their levels of general self-esteem, performace, acceptance, social confidence, and ethics on a
13 100-point scale where zero represented lower self-esteem than anyone else, 50 represented "average" self-esteem, and 100 represented better self-esteem than anyone. Then they were asked to rank the four component areas of self-esteem from one to four in terms of how important each area was in determining their overall level of self-esteem, one indicating the most important area, four indicating the least important area. Finally, the students were asked to read through the original questionnaire and select any items which they felt were especially relevant to their level of self-esteem; these items were then discussed, if time allowed. Throughout the interviews, the students were treated as intelligent human beings and co-investigators, whose ideas were at least as important as those of the investigator. Second questionnaire administration. The final phase of the study involved administering the perceived love and acceptance scale to four of the classes a second time, after an interval of five weeks; 88 students completed the scale. The scores of 41 students who completed both questionnaires and who had not been interviewed were used to calculate test- retest reliability of the "Perceived Love and Acceptance" scale.
14 RESULTS Acceptance Scale Of 146 students whose answers were tabulated, 145 responded to all the items of the "perceived love and acceptance" item pool. Since a preliminary factor analysis revealed that the structure of the responses was different for males than for females, the male and female subsamples were analyzed separately. By deleting, one at a time, items which correlated poorly with the overall scale score for the entire sample or for either subsample, a single scale was developed which exhibited high internal consistency for both groups. Of the original 21 items, 11 were eventually deleted, leaving ten items to form the final version of the scale. Ten of the deleted items correlated less than .40 with total scale score for the whole sample, or for one of the subsamples. One item, "Sometimes I feel like nobody notices if I am around or not," correlated very highly (r = .74) with a similar item, "Sometimes I feel like nobody cares if I am around or not." The former ("notices") was deleted in favor of the latter ("cares") which correlated more highly with total scale score. The final scale contained ten items, each of which correlated at least .40 with total scale score, whether looking at the entire sample, or at either of the two
15 Table 1 Item-total correlations and alpha coefficients for 10-item acceptance scale ------------------------------------------------------------- whole males females group only only Item (n = 145) (n = 50) (n = 88) ------------------------------------------------------------- 3 "no one interested in listening" .52 .61 .48 4 "most people accept me" .48 .59 .42 5 "I feel appreciated" .58 .54 .61 7 "nobody cares if I am around" .46 .56 .40 9 "my friends think I am fun" .42 .41 .41 12 "people find me interesting" .49 .45 .49 13 "someone understands me" .43 .45 .44 14 "I worry whether others like to be with me" .50 .50 .52 20 "people seem to like me" .63 .64 .62 21 "I feel left out" .63 .56 .67 -------------------------------------------------------------- alpha .82 .83 .81 -------------------------------------------------------------- subsamples. Cronbach alpha reliability for the final scale was .82. Table 1 shows the individual item-total correlations and alpha coefficients for both males and females. The responses of the subsamples were also subjected to separate factor analyses using principal factor analysis with iterations for communalities. The unrotated analyses yielded two factors with eigenvalues greater than one for males, and three factors with eigenvalues greater than one for females. Therefore, two-factor analyses were generated for the purpose of comparison. Since it was expected that the factors should be to some extent correlated, the factors were rotated to oblique simple structure. Table 2 shows the
16 Table 2 Factor pattern of the 10-item scale rotated to an oblique solution ------------------------------------------------------------- whole males females sample only only (n = 145) (n = 50) (n = 88) ------------------------------------------------------------- Factor Factor Factor Item 1 2 1 2 1 2 ------------------------------------------------------------- 3 "no one interested" .64 .02 .77 -.07 .64 .01 4 "people accept me" .72 -.12 .45 .27 .71 -.18 5 "I feel appreciated" .78 -.07 .49 .15 .81 -.05 7 "nobody cares" .09 .52 .42 .29 .02 .59 9 "think I am fun" -.05 .69 .06 .50 .07 .75 12 "find me interesting" -.02 .72 .28 .26 .04 .73 13 "someone understands" .50 .06 .52 .00 .56 .04 14 "I worry if others like to be with me" .42 .17 .71 -.14 .43 .21 20 "people like me" .39 .42 -.05 .99 .50 .32 21 "I feel left out" .45 .31 .47 .20 .46 .38 ------------------------------------------------------------- correlation between .54 .57 .41 factors ------------------------------------------------------------- oblique factor pattern matrices and inter-factor correlations for both males and females; the factor structures for the male and female subsamples are similar. Seven of the ten items loaded on the same factors in the matrices for both males and females. Items 7 and 12 loaded on factor 2 for females, on both factors for males. Item 20 loaded on factor 2 for males, on both factors for females. The two factors appear to be very similar. It may be that Factor 1, with such items as "There is no one who is really interested in listening to me," "Most people accept me for who I really am," and "I feel appreciated," deals with
17 how one feels he or she is being treated by other people. Factor 2, with such items as "My friends think I am a lot of fun" and "People usually find me an interesting person," may have more to do with the individual's acceptability or enjoyability as a person, regardless of how others might respond. Forty-one of the students completed the perceived love and acceptance scale a second time, five weeks after the administration of the original questionnaire. Item-total correlations and factor loadings were similar for the first and second administrations of the scale. Test-retest reliability of the ten-item scale for this sample was low, r = .62. This may be a characteristic of the scale; it may also be attributed to the students' changing self- perceptions. Several of the students who were interviewed stated that their responses had been influenced by recent experiences, e. g., breaking up with a boyfriend, having to choose a major in a hurry. Some of these same students indicated that their questionnaire responses were an accurate appraisal of how they had felt that day, but that they would have had to change their answers if queried at other times. Virtue Scale Construction of a unified scale measuring "virtue" utilizing the original pool of ten items proved to be
18 impossible. No combination of any number of items yielded item-total correlations greater than .45 or internal consistency reliability (coefficient alpha) greater than .55. Therefore, the attempt to construct a scale measuring "virtue" was abandoned. Instead, individual items from the pool were used in some of the analyses. The concept of a "virtue" construct did prove to be very important, as will be shown not only from the statistical analysis but even more clearly from the interviews. Shyness Scale The Stanford Shyness Survey was not published with the intent that it be used as a scale, and no procedure was given for scoring it as such. However, responses to the eight items used in this study proved to be very consistent internally, lending themselves to its use as a short scale measuring shyness. Some of the students did not answer all of these questions, because those classified as not being shy and never having been shy (on the basis of their responses to the first three questions of the Survey) were instructed to skip the rest of the questions as being irrelevant. Therefore, those who skipped these items were assigned low scores (indicating non-shyness) for each of the items. Item-total correlations ranged from .72 to .83 for the 5-item scale, and internal consistency reliability (Cronbach's
19 coefficient alpha) was .91. Figures for the male and female subsamples were very similar. Principal factor analysis of the 5-item scale yielded only one factor with eigenvalue greater than one; that factor accounted for more than 75 percent of the variance in the scale. When a two-factor solution was attempted, none of the items loaded on the second factor. This was also true of both the male and female subsamples. Differences based on Sex, Class Level, Age In one-way analyses of variance, none of the scale scores differed significantly between the sexes or between students of different class standings. When the sample was split in half at the median age (20-21 years), one-way analyses of variance revealed no significant differences between the younger and older groups in any of the scale scores. Self-Esteem Scale Interrelationships Table 3 shows the correlations between the global measure of self-esteem and each component area. These correlation coeficcients were tested using Williams' T2 test for significant differences between dependent coeficcients of correlation (Steiger, 1980), a modification of Hotelling's T1, but without the restrictive assumptions of T1. The male subsample exhibited no significant differences.
20 Table 3 Correlation of global self-esteem scale with self-esteem components ------------------------------------------------------------- Component Whole Group Males only Females only Scale (n = 146) (n = 50) (n = 89) ------------------------------------------------------------- Self-Efficacy .69* .71* .68* Social Behavior .46* .62* .48* Perceived Acceptance .67* .59* .70* ------------------------------------------------------------- * p < .001 However, for the whole sample as well as for females alone, the correlation between global self-esteem and social behavior was significantly lower than the correlation between self-esteem and self-efficacy; whole sample, T2 (143) = 3.34, p < .001; females only, T2 (86) = 2.235, p < 0.05. For the whole sample and for females, the correlation between self-esteem and social behavior was significantly lower than the correlation between self-esteen and perceived acceptance; whole sample, T2 (143) = 3.20, p < .01; females only, T2 (86) = 2.81, p < .01. (T2 has a t-distribution with df = n - 3). This indicates that, at least for females, self-esteem is much more related to performance and acceptance than to social behavior. However, the statistics for the whole sample may be misleading, since males did not exhibit these differences, and the preponderance of females in the sample may have skewed the whole-sample results somewhat.
21 Table 4 Correlations between self-esteem components ------------------------------------------------------------- Social Behavior Perceived Acceptance ------------------------------------------------------------- whole males females whole males females group only only group only only (n=146) (n=50) (n=89) (n=146) (n=50) (n=89) ------------------------------------------------------------- Self- Efficacy .35* .53* .41* .54* .57* .51* Social Behavior - - - .44* .61* .49* ------------------------------------------------------------- * p < .001 Table 4 shows the correlations between the self-esteem component scales. The correlations are moderate, indicating that the scales measure different, although related, dimensions. Since no "virtue" scale was constructed, the relationships between the various scales and the individual items from the virtue pool were examined instead. Table 5 shows that although several items from the pool correlated moderately with global self-esteem, none of them was as strong a predictor of global self-esteem as were the three scales of component areas of self-esteem. However, individual items would not be expected to correlate as highly as would complete scales. The relationships between the various self-esteem Table 5
22 Correlation of Self-esteem measures with virtue pool items ------------------------------------------------------------- Virtue Self- Self- Social Perceived Item Esteem Efficacy Behavior Acceptance ------------------------------------------------------------- 1 .23** .17* .06 .06 2 .42*** .38*** .27*** .27*** 3 .19* .10 .27*** .25*** 4 .17* .22** .03 .09 5 .08 .02 -.01 -.02 6 .13 .07 .09 .30*** 7 .29*** .25*** .26*** .25*** 8 .04 .12 .02 .19** 9 .26*** .23** .08 .26*** 10 .32*** .34*** .15* .33*** ------------------------------------------------------------- * p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001 measures were also examined by doing a principal factor analysis of the combined items from the global self-esteem, self-efficacy, social behavior, and perceived acceptance scales. Table 6 shows an analysis involving three factors rotated to orthogonal simple structure using Kaiser's varimax criterion. (When a four-factor analysis was attempted, few items loaded on the fourth factor.) All of the self-efficacy items loaded on one factor; most of the perceived acceptance items loaded on another factor; most of the social bahavior items loaded on the third factor. The global self-esteem items loaded on two factors -- primarily with the perceived acceptance items (factor 2), but also with the self-efficacy items (factor 1). None of the global self-esteem items loaded on the same factor with the social behavior items (factor 3). This concurred with the finding that the
23 Table 6 Factor analysis of four self-esteem measures rotated to Kaiser's Varimax criterion ------------------------------------------------------------- Scale Item Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 ------------------------------------------------------------- 1 .22 .56 .20 2 .40 .27 .11 E 3 .31 .41 .23 S 4 .33 .32 .15 T 5 .39 .38 .22 E 6 .45 .29 .10 E 7 .39 .38 .26 M 8 .39 .48 .17 9 .34 .67 .14 10 .39 .55 .16 ------------------------------------------------------------- 1 .35 .34 .15 2 .47 .25 .05 3 .60 .05 -.07 4 .46 .08 .04 E 5 .61 .04 .00 F 6 .60 .14 .24 F 7 .67 .15 .08 I 8 .48 .14 -.03 C 9 .39 .11 -.04 A 10 .58 .30 .01 C 11 .44 .36 .09 Y 12 .59 .28 .01 13 .38 .03 .04 14 .63 .45 .21 15 .69 .19 .20 16 .41 .42 .16 ------------------------------------------------------------- 1 .06 .18 .38 S 2 .28 .39 .57 O 3 .20 .47 .41 C 4 -.07 -.01 .66 I 5 .03 .02 .66 A 6 .21 .03 .66 L 7 .24 .02 .38 8 .19 -.01 .50 B 9 .12 .20 .55 E 10 -.25 -.01 .44 H 11 -.04 .05 .54 A 12 .00 .46 .37 V 13 .22 .02 .15 I 14 .00 .18 .45 O 15 .50 .16 .36 R 16 .08 .14 .69 ------------------------------------------------------------- A 3 .13 .58 .13 C 4 .06 .61 -.06 C 5 .14 .66 -.02 E 7 .26 .28 .29 P 9 .13 .29 .30 T 12 .25 .37 .32 A 13 .06 .62 -.05 N 14 .38 .50 .09 C 20 .16 .55 .12 E 21 .10 .58 .07 -------------------------------------------------------------
24 strongest predictors of global self-esteem were self-efficacy and perceived acceptance. However, the expected results were not entirely realized. It was expected that perceived acceptance would be the strongest predictor of global self- esteem. In fact, self-efficacy and perceived acceptance proved to be equally strong in predicting global self-esteem. Self-esteem and related variables It was expected that the Parental Interest Index, which has been shown to correlate with self-esteem in previous studies (Rosenberg,1965), would correlate with both global self-esteem and perceived acceptance. This turned out to be the case. Table 7 shows the correlations between the individual items of the Parental Interest Index and the various measures of self-esteem. All of the items of the Index correlated significantly with scores on the Love and Acceptance scale. Only three of the Index items correlated significantly with scores on the self-efficacy scale. The global self-esteem and social behavior scales each correlated significantly with four of the Index items. It was also expected that loneliness, which has been shown to correlate with self-esteem, would correlate most highly with perceived acceptance. This expectation was fully realized -- perhaps too fully. Table 8 shows that loneliness correlated more highly with acceptance than with any of the other self-esteem measures, but the relationship was strong
25 Table 7 Correlation of Self-esteem measures with Parental Interest Index items ------------------------------------------------------------- Self- Self- Social Perceived Interest item Esteem Efficacy Behavior Acceptance ------------------------------------------------------------- 1 mother knew friends .28*** .20** .05 .30*** 2 father knew friends .10 .02 .02 .17* 3 father--high grades .12 .11 .15* .19* 4 father--low grades .25** .14 .25** .22** 5 mother--high grades .12 .07 .22** .17* 6 mother--low grades .20* .15* .28*** .21** 7 conversation .31*** .27*** .14 .30*** ------------------------------------------------------------- * p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001 Table 8 Correlation between loneliness and various measures of self-esteem ------------------------------------------------------------- Self-esteem whole group males only females only measure (n=146) (n=50) (n=89) ------------------------------------------------------------- Self-esteem .59* .50* .66* Self-efficacy .44* .35* .53* Social Behavior .44* .47* .52* Perceived Acceptance .76* .68* .83* ------------------------------------------------------------- * p < .001 enough (r = .76) to raise questions whether the loneliness and acceptance scales were measuring two distinct dimensions or only one. The correlation was somewhat greater for females than for males. This difference in response patterns between males and females is amplified by the analysis in Table 9. The ten items of the acceptance scale and the 20
26 Table 9 Factor pattern of Acceptance and Loneliness items rotated to an oblique solution ------------------------------------------------------------- whole group males only females only (n = 146) (n = 50) (n = 89) ------------------------------------------------------------- Factor Factor Factor Scale Item 1 2 1 2 1 2 ------------------------------------------------------------- A 3 .21 .42 .20 .41 .17 .52 C 4 .26 .27 .15 .55 .15 .30 C 5 .48 .20 .13 .54 .38 .35 E 7 .59 .03 -.06 .64 .54 .04 P 9 .40 .24 .34 .41 .27 .32 T 12 .47 .09 -.20 .57 .39 .32 A 13 -.02 .69 .28 .35 -.08 .77 N 14 .56 -.05 .07 .50 .62 .04 C 20 .49 .19 -.04 .72 .36 .36 E 21 .73 -.07 .09 .57 .71 .01 ------------------------------------------------------------- 1 .40 .07 .09 .42 .28 .18 2 .45 .28 .58 .25 .48 .20 3 .10 .68 .71 .09 .18 .57 4 .02 .05 -.06 -.07 .09 .19 5 .40 .19 .53 .09 .58 .13 L 6 .46 .22 .42 .16 .53 .23 O 7 .13 .65 .53 .12 .15 .73 N 8 .63 -.04 .05 .43 .69 .01 E 9 -.04 .38 .41 -.07 -.17 .49 L 10 .05 .70 .47 .34 .06 .69 I 11 .76 -.17 .01 .52 .82 -.17 N 12 .23 .35 .53 .26 .22 .21 E 13 .22 .46 .53 .07 .34 .43 S 14 .53 .21 .58 .13 .71 .05 S 15 .13 .51 .41 .28 .08 .51 16 .06 .72 .80 -.11 .17 .65 17 .65 -.07 -.03 .52 .69 -.07 18 .63 .18 .52 .26 .76 .06 19 -.12 .91 .83 -.10 .00 .81 20 -.19 .94 .99 -.36 -.07 .87 ------------------------------------------------------------- Correlation Between .60 .46 .57 Factors -------------------------------------------------------------
27 items of the loneliness scale were subjected to a principal factor analysis, rotated to an oblique solution. A two- factor solution failed to distinguish between the two measures for females, but for males, all of the acceptance items loaded on one factor, while 15 of the 20 loneliness items loaded on the second factor. It was expected that shyness would correlate most highly with the acceptance and social behavior scales. This turned out to be only partly true, as seen in Table 10. For males, shyness correlated moderately with social behavior, but only weakly with acceptance. On the other hand, both Table 10 Correlation between shyness and various measures of self-esteem ------------------------------------------------------------- Self-Esteem whole group males only females only measure (n = 146) (n = 50) (n = 89) ------------------------------------------------------------- Self-Esteem .28*** .36** .23* Self-Efficacy .13 .31* .01 Social Behavior .25*** .48*** .19* Perceived Acceptance .22** .26* .20* Loneliness .41*** .43*** .38*** ------------------------------------------------------------- * p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001
28 social behavior and acceptance correlated only weakly with shyness for females. An interesting observation is that for males, shyness correlated statistically significantly with self-efficacy (r = .31, p < .05), while for females the correlation was zero. Interview Data Construct validity. The twenty-three students who were interviewed were asked to rate themselves on the dimensions of "overall self-esteem," "performance" (self- efficacy), "social confidence," "acceptance," and "ethics." The correlations between these oral self-ratings and scores on the paper-and-pencil scales appear in Table 11. The construct validity of the global self-esteem, self-efficacy, and perceived acceptance scales appears to be indicated by the high correlations between the ratings and the scale scores. However, for Performance and Social Confidence, the oral self-ratings correlated more highly with a different scale than with the scale of the same construct. It may be that some those interviewed were confused about exactly what they were being instructed to rate. Some of the students seemed to have difficulty distinguishing between social behavior and acceptance, others between global self-esteem and performance.
29 Table 11 Correlations between scale scores and oral self-ratings ------------------------------------------------------------- Oral self-ratings ------------------------------------------------------------- Self- Social Scale Esteem Performance Confidence Acceptance ------------------------------------------------------------- Self- Esteem .63*** .48* .31 .51** Self- Efficacy .50* .53** .04 .33 Social Behavior .51** .56** .01 .22 Perceived Acceptance .43** .45* .31 .60*** ------------------------------------------------------------- * p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001 Sources of self-esteem. The students were asked to describe the various things that made them feel good about themselves and the things that made them feel bad about themselves. These 23 students named approximately 167 things that made them feel good about themselves, and approximately 123 things that made them feel bad about themselves. Summaries of these responses appear in Tables 12 and 13. Since the responses could be understood and categorized in various ways, the numbers in this section are understood to be approximate. Table 12 shows that activities that define the performance area dominate what these students considered to
30 contribute to their good self-esteem, with 72 references to such activities. The acceptance area received about half as many references, 38. The virtue area received half as many again, 19 references. The area of social skills was mentioned least, with only 14 references. A fifth area, Table 12 Items contributing to good self-esteem as discussed in personal interviews with university students (summary of approximately 167 responses of 23 students) ------------------------------------------------------------ A. Performance Area . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72 1. Achieving goals, being productive . . . . 19 2. Grades, academic achievement . . . . . . . 16 3. Talents (art, music, cooking, etc.) . . . 16 4. Participating in athletics . . . . . . . . . 9 5. Going to university . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 6. Overcoming smoking, depression, etc. . . . . 4 7. Other performance-related references . . . . 8 B. Acceptance Area . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 1. Having loving family and friends, having the approval of others, being appreciated, being popular . . . . . 24 2. Receiving compliments, praise, attention . . 9 3. Other acceptance-related references . . . . 5 C. Virtue Area . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19 1. Helping people . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11 2. Making others feel good . . . . . . . . . . 8 D. Social Area . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14 1. Getting along with people . . . . . . . . 10 2. Being able to understand people . . . . . . 2 3. Not being shy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 E. Personal Appearance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12 F. Other miscellaneous references . . . . . . . . . . . 12 -------------------------------------------------------------
31 personal appearance, was mentioned 12 times. Table 13 shows that the pattern was somewhat different for items contributing to poor self-esteem. The acceptance, virtue, and performance areas all carried about equal weight, being mentioned 30, 27, and 26 times, respectively. Again, personal appearance was not mentioned as often, only 18 times, and social skills were referred to only 13 times. Table 13 Items contributing to poor self-esteem as discussed in personal interviews with university students (summary of approximately 123 responses of 23 students) ------------------------------------------------------------- A. Acceptance Area . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30 1. Being looked down upon or put down . . . . 18 2. Being ignored, not being recognized . . . 12 B. Virtue Area . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27 1. Not getting along with people, being mean, angry, holding grudges . . . . 16 2. Letting people down . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 3. Being selfish, impatient, manipulating . . . 7 C. Performance Area . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26 1. Being lazy, procrastinating, having no ambition, not achieving goals . . . . . 18 2. Not doing well in school, sports . . . . . . 6 3. Having a chosen career prevented by inadequate performance . . . . . . . . . . . 2 D. Personal Appearance . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18 E. Social Area . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13 1. Social ineptitude, shyness, lack of assertiveness, need for tact . . . . . . . . 8 2. Bad experiences in dating or relationships . 4 3. Unjustified feelings of inferiority . . . . 1 F. Other miscellaneous references . . . . . . . . . . . 9 -------------------------------------------------------------
32 In addition to naming various things that made them feel good about themselves and things that made them feel bad about themselves, the students were also asked to rank the various items that they had selected, from most important to least important. Table 14 shows the total number of references to various areas compared with the number of Table 14 Number of items contributing to good self-esteem and poor self-esteem categorized by area ------------------------------------------------------------- items contributing to good self-esteem ------------------------------------------------------------- times times selected area selected as most important ------------------------------------------------------------- Performance 76 21 Acceptance 38 14 Virtue 19 9 Social 14 8 Appearance 12 5 Other areas 10 6 ------------------------------------------------------------- items contributing to poor self-esteem ------------------------------------------------------------- times times selected area selected as most important ------------------------------------------------------------- Acceptance 31 16 Virtue 27 14 Performance 26 11 Appearance 18 9 Social 14 3 Other areas 10 2 ------------------------------------------------------------- references selected as the three most important. This comparison shows that the ranking of the items was virtually
33 identical whether all references were included, or only those items considered most important by the individual students. Finally, after a discussion of the four self-esteem areas under consideration, the students were asked to rank the importance of each of the four areas in determining their overall level of self-esteem, either good or bad. These rankings are shown in Table 15. Almost half the students ranked performance first. Virtue was generally ranked first or second, and acceptance was generally ranked second or third. The social skills area was ranked least important, with almost half the students giving it fourth place out of four areas. Table 15 How 23 university students ranked four areas as contributing to their good self-esteem or poor self-esteem ------------------------------------------------------------ ranked as ranked as most ranked ranked least mean area important second third important rank ------------------------------------------------------------- Performance 11 5 4 3 1.96 Virtue 9 7 3 4 2.09 Acceptance 3 7 7 6 2.70 Social 1 3 9 10 3.22 -------------------------------------------------------------
34 DISCUSSION Dimensions of Self-Esteem The current study certainly substantiates the importance of perceived acceptance as an element of self-esteem, an element which is somewhat distinct from the other dimensions of performance and social confidence. The Rosenberg Self- Esteem Scale correlates most highly with the dimensions of self-esteem characterized by (1) generalized expectations of competence and (2) feelings of being loved and accepted. For males, the Rosenberg scale also correlated highly with a third dimension, social confidence. This difference between the sexes may be explained by taking a closer look at the Texas Social Behavior Inventory, which was used to measure social confidence. Nine of its 16 items concern initiative, leadership, or assertiveness -- traditionally masculine values. The cultivation of these characteristics may be more important to males than to females, for whom other social characteristics, e.g., harmony and sensitivity, may be more valued. If this is true, we would expect the exhibiting of these traditionally masculine characteristics to have more of an effect upon the self-esteem of males than upon the self-esteem of females. The fact that the distinction between perceived acceptance and loneliness was somewhat blurred should not be considered a drawback. There are two possible ways to deal with this information. The first is to continue to affirm
35 that perceived acceptance and loneliness are indeed distinct constructs and to focus efforts on developing scales for each which clarify their differences. It may be that the acceptance scale contains items which really measure loneliness; it may also be that the UCLA Loneliness Scale contains several items which really refer to feelings of rejection. The fact that males respond to the two scales as if they measure different dimensions is encouraging. On the other hand, if perceived rejection and loneliness are indeed different ways of saying the same thing, then we have both increased our understanding of loneliness and affirmed that for some people, a primary source of poor self-esteem is feelings of loneliness. It is unfortunate that the concept of "virtue" could not be analyzed as fully as was desired, because of the inability to produce a scale measuring the dimension. However, its importance as an element of self-esteem was established by the responses of the 23 students who were interviewed, who ranked the personal ethics area second only to the performace area as important in determining their self- esteem. The question which remains is, how do we define virtue or personal ethics? When allowed to freely express their perceived sources of self-esteem, these students did not raise issues of law or religion, but most often spoke of interpersonal relations -- helping people, making others feel
36 good, not being mean, not holding grudges, and so on (see Tables 12 and 13). However, when asked to select questionnaire items most relevant to their self-esteem, the issues of personal and professional honesty came up Table 16 Number of students who selected various virtue pool items as being relevant to their self-esteem (total number of students = 23) ------------------------------------------------------------- item times selected item times selected ------------------------------------------------------------- 1 7 6 3 2 2 7 1 3 0 8 7 4 5 9 3 5 2* 10 2 ------------------------------------------------------------- * Number 5, "I consider myself more religious than the average American," was specifically singled out by several students as being unimportant to their self-esteem. No other items was singled out in this way. frequently. Table 16 shows how many students selected each of the virtue pool items as being relevant to their self- esteem. The most frequently selected items were number 1, "I pride myself on being honest," number 4, "I often become upset when I see business conducted in a manner which is not ethical," and number 8, "It is important to me that I try to be helpful to others." It appears that for these students, "virtue" consists of twin elements: consideration for others and interpersonal integrity. A scale or scales measuring these areas might be useful.
37 The Development of Self-Esteem Inasmuch as parental interest is considered to be a primary antecedent or source of self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965; Coopersmith, 1967), the information in Table 7 takes on a special meaning. This table shows how the various items of the Parental Interest Index (Rosenberg, 1965) correlate with the various self-esteem measures. The Index taps three different areas: (1) the parents' interest in the child's friends and activities, items 1 and 2; (2) the parents' interest in the child's academic achievement, items 3 through 6; and (3) the family's interest in the child's conversation, item 7. Interest in the child's academic achievement was more highly correlated with social confidence than with perceived task competence. On the other hand, item 7, the family's interest in the child's conversation, was not related to social confidence, but correlated significantly with every other area of self-esteem. This seems to be exactly the opposite of what we would expect -- which would be that expressing interest in the child's academic performance would influence his or her perception of task competence, and that listening to the child would influence his or her social skills. All of the Parental Interest Index items correlated significantly with perceived acceptance, as was expected. It may be that different parental behaviors affect different areas of the child's development and future
38 self-perception, but research in this area must be left to studies of actual parental behaviors rather than distant memories of them. The preceeding discussion of the development of self- esteem concerns the evolution of the state of the individual's self-esteem. Another area which may now be addressed is the evolution of the structure of the individual's self-esteem. Does the importance of each of the self-esteem elements change as the individual develops? Does one start life basing self-esteem on acceptance by others, but eventually grow independent enough to base self-esteem on meeting personal ethical standards and performance goals (as was suggested by one of the interviewed students)? It might be suggested (and not without reason) that the small child derives feelings of self-worth from unconditional acceptance by his or her parents, the school-aged child from academic performance, the young adult from the social skills used in dating and mate selection, and the mature adult from maintaining high standards of personal and professional integrity. Statements of those interviewed would support this statement. The correlation between the student's age and ranking of personal ethics as contributing to his or her self-esteem was moderate but statistically significant, r = -.39, p = .032. This indicates that the tendency to rank personal ethics as most important increased with age. A one-
39 way analysis of variance showed a significant difference between (1) 18- and 19-year olds and (2) those over 19 years old in their rankings of personal ethics, F(1,21) = 10.032, p = .005). While none of the other self-esteem elements showed such a relationship, this certainly indicates that the area is worthy of further research. Directions for future research It is essential that several deficiencies of the present study be remedied. First, the distinct response patterns of males and females to the perceived acceptance items make it necessary that more males complete the questionnaire, in order that adequate analyses can be done. The present study utilized about twice as many females as males; thus, any results for the total sample may have been skewed in the direction of female responses. Second, the pool of perceived acceptance items needs to be increased in size and narrowed in scope, so that (if possible) rejection may be distinguished from loneliness. Third, the pool of virtue items also needs to be increased in size and narrowed in scope, so that a consistent scale might be produced. The most logical next step is to examine the relationships between these variables in different age groups. Some of the scales used in this study may be
40 unsuitable for use with children, and other scales would have to be substituted. However, this investigator's experience with 23 students ranging in age from 18 to 45 indicates that even among adults there are many differences which might be attributed to increasing age and personal development. Perhaps a study of adults in age-decades would be appropriate. Another area of interest might be to try to determine the antecedents of each of the self-esteem elements, an issue which has been discussed briefly already. Personal interviews would again be one excellent way in which to conduct such research, although a better study would attempt to determine the relationships between actual parental behaviors (and other factors believed to contribute to the development of self-esteem) and the various elements of self- esteem. Interviews involve perceptions and memories of the past, possibly distorted; observational methods deal with actual behaviors, although possibly divorced from their impact on the individual in question. The best study would include both types of data. Final Note The information presented in this study could have been gathered without the use of personal interviews. However, the following distinct advantages of speaking personally with some of the students should be noted.
41 (1) Most of the information collected in the interviews could have been collected by paper-and-pencil means, but it would have been extremely difficult to do. Some of the answers, particularly the totally free responses to the question, "What makes you feel good about yourself?," would not have been as complete and clear if answered in writing. (2) Speaking to the students personally gave the investigator an opportunity to fully explain his questions and the information he was looking for. It turned out that several of these students had misunderstood many questionnaire items and had answered inappropriately. There was less room for such confusion to arise in the interview sessions. (3) The students' oral statements served to correct the investigator's misconceptions about the meaning of responses to the questionnaire items. In many types of social research, the investigator must make inferences about the subjects' thoughts from their written responses. In an ethogenic approach to social psychological research (Harre, 1977), the subjects are allowed to explain their actions, in this case consisting of written responses to questionnaire items. This decreases the need for making certain types of inferences, although it does not totally eliminate this need. However, it must be remembered that the subjects' inferences about the reasons for their actions may also influence their
42 actions, while the investigator's inferences about the causes of their behavior, unless expressed, have no influence on the behavior of the subjects. This is of particular importance in a study of self-perception. (4) Finally, the interviews had beneficial effects on both the students and the investigator. Although several of the students came to their interview sessions reluctantly, because they felt shy or were busy studying, every one of them indicated that they had come because they wanted to make a contribution to research in this area and because they found it interesting. Each of them left feeling that they had been really listened to, and had made unique contributions to the study. As concerns the investigator, the interviews encouraged him to remember that the study was not about number-2 pencil marks on bubble sheets, but about people, and their deepest thoughts and feelings. This investigator reccomends this avenue of social psychological research to anyone who is really interested in getting to the bottom of the how's and why's of the ways we human beings think and feel.
43 SUMMARY Self-esteem can be thought of as consisting of several distinct factors or elements. It has been suggested that one element of self-esteem might be the individual's perception of being loved and accepted by others (Coopersmith, 1967). Such a factor would probably show high correlations with an individual's social behaviors and attitudes. But no measure has been constructed which assesses this important element of self-esteem. The purpose of this study was to create a self- report scale measuring "Perceived Love and Acceptance," and to determine its correlation with measures of "Parental Interest," shyness and loneliness, all of which have been shown to correlate with general self-esteem (Rosenberg, 1965; Coopersmith, 1967). One hundred forty-six university students responded to written self-report inventories of parental interest, shyness, general self-esteem, and four specific elements of self-esteem, including "Perceived Love and Acceptance." Scores on the "Perceived Love and Acceptance" scale were moderately predictive of scores on the general self-esteem scale. In private interviews, 23 students described their self-esteem in terms of three major components: performance, perceived acceptance, and personal ethics. The need for further research of these components is suggested.
44 REFERENCES Battle, J. Relationship between self-esteem and depression among high school students. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1980, 51, 157-158. Carmines, E. G., & Zeller, R. A. On establishing the empirical dimensionality of theoretical items: an analytic example. Political Methodology, 1974, 1, 75- 96. Coopersmith, S. The antecedents of self-esteem. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and Co., 1967. Dean, D. Alienation: Its meaning and measurement. American Sociological Review, 1961, 26, 753-758. DeGregorio, E., & Carver, C. S. Type A behavior pattern, sex-role orientation, and psychological adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1980, 39, 286-293. Fey, W. F. Acceptance by others and its relation to acceptance of self and others: A reevaluation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1955, 50, 274-276. Gecas, V. Parental behavior and dimensions of adolescent self-evaluation. Sociometry, 1971, 34, 466-482. Harre, R. The ethogenic approach: theory and practice. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 10. New York: Academic Press, 1977. Helmreich, R. & Stapp, J. Short forms of the Texas Social Behavior Inventory (TSBI), an objective measure of self- esteem. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 1974, 4, 473-475. Jacobs, L., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. Self-esteem and attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1971, 17, 84-91. Lasky, E. Physical attractiveness and its relationship to self-esteem: preliminary findings. In M. Cook & G. Wilson (Eds.), Love and attraction. Oxford: Pergamon Press, 1979.
45 Nie, N. H., Hull, C. H., Jenkins, J. G., Steinbrenner, K., & Bent, D. H. SPSS: Statistical package for the social sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1975. Perlman, D. & Peplau, L. A. Toward a social psychology of loneliness. In S. Duck & R. Gilmour (Eds.), Personal relationships. 3. Personal relationships in disorder. London: Academic Press, 1981. Petrie, K. & Rotheram, M. J. Insulators against stress: self-esteem and assertiveness. Psychological Reports, 1982, 50, 963-966. Pilkonis, P. A., & Zimbardo, P. G. The personal and social dynamics of shyness. In C. E. Izard (Ed.), Emotions is personality and psychotherapy. New York: Plenum Press, 1979. Robinson, J. P., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.) Measures of social psychological attitudes. Ann Arbor, Michigan: Institute for Social Research, 1973. Rosenberg, M. Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, N. J.: Princeton University Press, 1965. Rosenberg, M. Conceiving the Self. New York: Basic Books, 1979. Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. The Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1980, 39, 472-480. Sherer, M., Maddux, J. E., Mercandante, B., Prentice-Dunn, S., Jacobs, B., & Rogers, R. W. The self-efficacy scale: Construction and validation. Psychological Reports, 1982, 51, 663-671. Silber, E., & Tippett, J. S. Self-esteem: Clinical assessment and measurement validation. Psychological Reports, 1965, 16, 1017-1071. Steiger, J. H. Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 1980, 87, 245-251. Thomas, C. B. Stamina: the thread of human life. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 1982, 38, 74-80.
46 Wilson, A. R., & Krane, R. V. Change in self-esteem and its effects on symptoms in depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1980, 4, 419-421. Zimbardo, P. G. Shyness. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1977.
47 APPENDIX A ROSENBERG SELF-ESTEEM SCALE Please indicate the extent of your agreement or disagreement with each of the following ten statements, by circling the appropriate responses to the right. SA = strongly agree A = agree D = disagree SD = strongly disagree 1. On the whole, I am satisfied with myself. SA A D SD 2. At times I think I am no good at all. SA A D SD 3. I feel that I have a number of good qualities. SA A D SD 4. I am able to do things as well as most other people. SA A D SD 5. I feel I do not have much to be proud of. SA A D SD 6. I certainly feel useless at times. SA A D SD 7. I feel that I am a person of worth, at least on an equal plane with others. SA A D SD 8. I wish I could have more respect for myself. SA A D SD 9. All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure. SA A D SD 10. I take a positive attitude toward myself. SA A D SD
48 APPENDIX B SELF-EFFICACY SCALE Please indicate the extent of your agreement or disagreement with each of the following statements by circling the appropriate response to the right of each item. SA = strongly agree A = agree D = disagree SD = strongly disagree 1. When I make plans, I am certain I can make them work. SA A D SD 2. One of my problems is that I cannot get down to work when I should. SA A D SD 3. If I can't do a job the first time, I keep trying until I can. SA A D SD 4. When I set important goals for myself, I rarely achieve them. SA A D SD 5. I give up on things before completing them. SA A D SD 6. I avoid facing difficulties. SA A D SD 7. If something looks too complicated, I will not even bother to try it. SA A D SD 8. When I have something unpleasant to do, I stick to it until I finish it. SA A D SD 9. When I decide to do something, I go right to work on it. SA A D SD 10. When trying to learn something new, I soon give up if I am not initially successful. SA A D SD 11. When unexpected problems occur, I don't handle them well. SA A D SD 12. A avoid trying to learn new things when they look too difficult for me. SA A D SD
49 13. Failure just makes me try harder. SA A D SD 14. I feel insecure about my ability to do things. SA A D SD 15. I give up easily. SA A D SD 16. I do not seem capable of dealing with most problems that come up in life. SA A D SD
50 APPENDIX C TEXAS SOCIAL BEHAVIOR INVENTORY (FORM A) Please indicate how true of you the following statements are of you, by placing the appropriate number in each of the blanks. 1 = not at all characteristic of me 2 = not very characteristic of me 3 = slightly characteristic of me 4 = fairly characteristic of me 5 = very much characteristic of me _____ 1. I am not likely to speak to people until they speak to me. _____ 2. I would describe myself as self-confident. _____ 3. I feel confident of my appearance. _____ 4. I am a good mixer. _____ 5. When in a group of people, I have trouble thinking of the right things to say. _____ 6. When in a group of people, I ususally do what the others want rather than make suggestions. _____ 7. When I am in disagreement with other people, my opinion usually prevails. _____ 8. I would describe myself as one who attempts to master situations. _____ 9. Other people look up to me. _____ 10. I enjoy social gatherings just to be with people. _____ 11. I make a point of looking other people in the eye. _____ 12. I cannot seem to get others to notice me. _____ 13. I would rather not have very much responsibility for other people. _____ 14. I feel comfortable being approached by someone in a position of authority. _____ 15. I would describe myself as indecisive. _____ 16. I have no doubts about my social competence.
51 APPENDIX D ITEMS FOR LOVE AND ACCEPTANCE SCALE Please indicate the extent of your agreement or disagreement with each of the following statements by circling the appropriate response to the right of each item. SA = strongly agree A = agree N = neither agree nor disagree D = disagree SD = strongly disagree 1. I am part of a group where I am made to feel important. SA A N D SD 2. I dread meeting new people because I know they will not like me. SA A N D SD 3. There is no one who is really interested in listening to me. SA A N D SD 4. Most people accept me for who I really am. SA A N D SD 5. I feel appreciated. SA A N D SD 6. Sometimes I feel like nobody notices if I am around or not. SA A N D SD 7. Sometimes I feel like nobody cares if I am around or not. SA A N D SD 8. I'm glad I have someone who really loves me. SA A N D SD 9. My friends think I am a lot of fun. SA A N D SD 10. I wish people would pay more attention to me. SA A N D SD 11. I still hurt because I have been rejected by someone important to me. SA A N D SD 12. People usually find me an interesting person. SA A N D SD 13. I'm glad there is someone who really unerstands me. SA A N D SD 14. I worry about whether other people like to be with me. SA A N D SD
52 15. It is hard for me to trust anyone. SA A N D SD 16. There is someone in my life whose happiness means as much to me as my own. SA A N D SD 17. There are people I can always count on. SA A N D SD 18. I don't get invited out by friends as often as i'd really like. SA A N D SD 19. People are quite critical of me. SA A N D SD 20. People seem to like me. SA A N D SD 21. I feel "left out," as if people don't want me around. SA A N D SD
53 APPENDIX E ITEMS FOR VIRTUE SCALE Please indicate the extent of your agreement or disagreement with each of the following statements by circling the appropriate response to the right of each item. SA = strongly agree A = agree N = neither agree nor disagree D = disagree SD = strongly disagree 1. I pride myself on being honest. SA A N D SD 2. I wish I were more dependable. SA A N D SD 3. I try to be concerned about other people, but it's hard to avoid being pretty self-centered. SA A N D SD 4. I often become upset when I see business conducted in a manner which is not ethical. SA A N D SD 5. I consider myself more religious than the average American. SA A N D SD 6. I wish I could be more gentle; sometimes I am too quick-tempered. SA A N D SD 7. I often feel guilty about holding grudges. SA A N D SD 8. It is important to me that I try to be helpful to others. SA A N D SD 9. I am pretty considerate of others. SA A N D SD 10. It is difficult for me to behave in a manner consistent with my beliefs. SA A N D SD
54 APPENDIX F STANFORD SHYNESS SURVEY _____ 1. Do you consider yourself to be a shy person? 1 = yes 2 = no _____ 2. If yes, have you always been shy (were shy previously and still are)? 1 = yes 2 = no _____ 3. If no to question 1, was there ever a prior time in your life when you were shy? 1 = yes 2 = no If no, then you are finished with this survey. Thanks. If yes to any of the above, please continue. _____ 4. How shy are you when you feel shy? 1 = extremely shy 2 = very shy 3 = quite shy 4 = moderately shy 5 = somewhat shy 6 = only slightly shy _____ 5. How often do you experience (have you experienced) these feelings of shyness? 1 = every day 2 = almost every day 3 = often, nearly every other day 4 = one or two times a week 5 = occasionally, less than once a week 6 = rarely, once a month or less _____ 6. Compared to your peers (of similar age, sex and background), how shy are you? 1 = much more shy 2 = more shy 3 = about as shy 4 = less shy 5 = much less shy _____ 7. How desirable is it for you to be shy? 1 = very undesirable 2 = undesirable 3 = neither 4 = desirable 5 = very desirable
56 _____ 8. Is (or was) your shyness ever a personal problem for you? 1 = yes, often 2 = yes, sometimes 3 = yes, occasionally 4 = rarely 5 = never
57 APPENDIX G PARENTAL INTEREST INDEX 1. When you were about 10-11 years old, did your mother know most of your friends? _____ knew all of my friends _____ knew most of my friends _____ knew some of my friends _____ knew almost none of my friends _____ knew none of my friends 2. During this period, did your father know your friends? _____ knew all of my friends _____ knew most of my friends _____ knew some of my friends _____ knew almost none of my friends _____ knew none of my friends 3. When you were in the 5th/6th grades, did your father usually pay attention when you brought home a report card with high grades? _____ yes _____ no 4. What if you brought home low grades? _____ yes _____ no 5. When you were in the 5th/6th grades, did you mother usually pay attention when you brought home a report card with high grades? _____ yes _____ no 6. What if you brought home low grades? _____ yes _____ no 7. As far as you can tell, how interested were other family members in what you had to say? _____ very interested _____ fairly interested _____ not interested
58 APPENDIX H REVISED UCLA LONELINESS SCALE Directions: Indicate how often you feel the way described in the following statements by circling the appropriate response to the right of each item. N = never R = rarely S = sometimes A = always 1. I feel in tune with the people around me. N R S A 2. I lack companionship. N R S A 3. There is no one I can turn to. N R S A 4. I do not feel alone. N R S A 5. I feel part of a group of friends. N R S A 6. I have a lot in common with the people around me. N R S A 7. I am no longer close to anyone. N R S A 8. My interests and ideas are not shared by those around me. N R S A 9. I am an outgoing person. N R S A 10. There are people I feel close to. N R S A 11. I feel left out. N R S A 12. My social relationships are superficial. N R S A 13. No one really knows me well. N R S A 14. I feel isolated from others. N R S A 15. I can find companionship when I want it. N R S A 16. There are people who really understand me. N R S A
59 17. I am unhappy being so withdrawn. N R S A 18. People are around me but not with me. N R S A 19. There are people I can talk to. N R S A 20. There are people I can turn to. N R S A
60 APPENDIX J ATTITUDE SURVEY This is a survey of your attitudes about yourself, other people, and various situations. It will take about one-half hour to complete the survey. As you answer the questions, try to be as honest as possible. There are no right or wrong answers; neither is any one answer more desirable than another. Your initial response to each question is usually the best indication of how you really feel, so don't spend too much time on any one question. The answers you give may provide an important contribution to social research. Enjoy being in the limelight! Please answer the questions on one page completely before going on to the next page; once you have completed a page, do not look back to see how you answered a question, or to change your answers. Thank you for investing your time in this project! Class ______________________________________________________ Day/Time ___________________________________________________ Instructor _________________________________________________ Code Number ________________________________________________ If you would like to be interviewed in connection with this project, please fill out the rest of this page. Name __________________________ Telephone Number ___________ When would you be free to be interviewed? __________________ ____________________________________________________________
61 APPENDIX K INFORMED CONSENT FOR INTERVIEW You are invited to participate in a study of self-esteem. We hope to learn more about what self-esteem is and how it develops. You were selected as a participant in the study because you expressed an interest in participating. If you decide to participate, Mark Brautigam, under the supervision of Dr. Milton Andersen, will interview you, asking questions about selected areas of your personality. Some of the questions may seem quite personal, and may cause some embarrassment or discomfort. However, it is not required of you that you answer any question if you do not want to; if you decide to participate, you are free to withdraw your consent and to discontinue participation at any time. We believe that the potential risk of embarrassment is far outweighed by the potential benefits of increased self- awareness and enhanced self-esteem. Any information that is obtained in connection with this study and that can be identified with you will remain confidential and will be disclosed only with your permission. If you have any questions, we expect you to ask us. If you have any additional questions later, Mark Brautigam (578-9132) or Dr. Milton Andersen (277-2615) will be happy to answer them. YOU ARE MAKING A DECISION WHETHER OR NOT TO PARTICIPATE. YOUR SIGNATURE INDICATES THAT YOU HAVE DECIDED TO PARTICIPATE HAVING READ THE INFORMATION PROVIDED ABOVE. __________________________ ______________________________ Date Signature ______________________________ Signature of Investigator